I don't know how audible these effects are. My understanding is that frequency response in the acoustic nearfield can change with distance, unlike when in the farfield there is only sound level attenuation. How much of it is audible? I have little idea. We know that because of room reflections, there is already a lot of comb filtering going on in a room that causes FR variations at differing listening locations, and our hearing is not very sensitive to them -- the reason we often do spatial averaging to smooth these variations out when measuring in-room FR.
Also, by definition, we are usually listening in the acoustic nearfield at low frequencies. What difference does that make, and do we notice?
Yeah. I also think Klippel's definition is very conservative. When you look at the plot in Amir's Lyd 5 review, the "Total Apparent Power" curve levels off a long way before 1.54 m. But it is very difficult to tell the actual trend with a vertical scale of -100 dB to >200 dB. The criterion Klippel uses is the point where "Total Apparent Power" levels off to within 0.5 dB of the asymptote.
I don't know if there is an ideal room size. I'll quote Dr Toole's paper.
AES E-Library ← Back to search
www.aes.org
He was talking about speaker directivity in this paper, but I think it is applicable also to non-reflective vs reflective rooms or direct field vs reverberant field listening. When reflections are minimized, we get more pin-point imaging, but at the expense of losing spaciousness. So there is a delicate balance, which likely depends heavily on individual preference.
View attachment 171179