Thank you! This looks to me remarkably like an unsmoothed version of the so-called Shirley curve or dip (Shirley, B.G., Kendrick, P., and Churchill, C. (2007). “The Effect of Stereo Crosstalk on Intelligibility: Comparison of a Phantom Stereo Image and a Central Loudspeaker Source,” J. Audio Eng. Soc., 55, pp. 852–863), also I mentioned here https://www.audiosciencereview.com/...nd-speaker-reviews.51091/page-18#post-1845124, but I'll reproduce here (sorry!):J J has assigned us some homework, and I am not going to reap the full benefits without doing the homework. I hope, by not ignoring his suggestions, I can also encourage J J to keep educating us. I am also aware that some members here are not familiar with the math, and hopefully this may help them too.
...
And here are the differences in the FR magnitudes between the phantom center and real center, displaying the phantom center dip J J mentioned.
View attachment 393949
@j_j Could you please explain why this floor-bounce range as per Klippel measurements and what you mentioned is different from what I thought was traditional audiophile understanding of bass frequencies below, say, 200-400 Hz being affected by destructive interference by the first floor reflection in typical listening rooms with typical speaker and listener heights?You get an A+ in my book. Very nicely done. Notice how that "dip" is smack in the ITD range, too, as well as the floor-bounce range. This is part of why the distance cues get squashed in a 2-channel setting.
Also notice that there are effectively two peaks in the "phantom center" HRIR sum, and that's part of what mucks up the senses of distance, as well, since it's in the "envelope sensitivity" area.
Also, @NTK 's graph shows a much higher peak around 8 kHz. Could that in the context of so-called Blauert bands at least partly explain why phantom center images seem to be typically perceived as elevated in the vertical plane (my perception, also Choisel, S., and Wickelmaier, F. (2007). “Evaluation of Multichannel Reproduced Sound: Scaling Auditory Attributes Underlying Listener Preferences,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 121, pp. 388–400.)
Thanks!