The question of whether blind people can "hear more" than sighted individuals is complex and involves several factors, including sensory compensation and individual differences in perception.
- Sensory Compensation: Some studies suggest that, because blind individuals rely more on their other senses (particularly hearing), they may develop enhanced abilities in those senses. This phenomenon is often attributed to neuroplasticity, where the brain adapts to the loss of one sense by enhancing the others. For example, blind individuals may have better auditory skills, including the ability to detect slight variations in sound, localize sounds, or even interpret auditory information more effectively.
- Training and Experience: Blind individuals may also have training and experience that allow them to use their auditory senses more effectively. Techniques developed in orientation and mobility training often rely heavily on sound, which can lead to improved listening skills.
- Perception of Sound: Blind individuals may perceive sound differently, often focusing on nuances and details that sighted individuals may overlook. This does not necessarily mean they hear more sounds, but rather that their interpretation of sounds can be more acute.
- Individual Variability: Just like sighted individuals, hearing abilities among blind individuals can vary widely due to genetics, environment, and personal experiences.
In summary, while many blind people may develop enhanced auditory skills, it is not correct to say universally that they hear "more" than sighted individuals. Their abilities may be different, and often such comparisons depend on advanced training and experience rather than an inherent advantage in hearing.
Echolocation
It sounds amazing, but we can all learn to use sound to detect our surroundings, just like bats or dolphins. No eyes required.
phys.org
Blinds echolocation reveals the importance of reflexes in rooms. Blind people can use echolocation to cycle in traffic. Higher frequencies are crucial for creating a sound image in real time. Through brain placidity areas of the brain, which are normally used for visual impressions in the sighted, are used to create a sequential 3D sound image of the surroundings in the blind human.
Just like bats, seeing and blind humans can use echolokcation
The importance of correct reflexes is underestimated.
Gelenec believes that all reflexes (above 1000 Hz), mainly laterally, must not arrive before 20 ms in sound studios relative to the direct sound. Reflexes over 20 ms are then usually attenuated by approx. 8 dB and add a spatially positive effect. Reflexes shorter than 20 ms and with a loudness greater than - 8 dB have a negativ spatially effect. Short reflexes add masking and perceptual distorting effects.
Echo localization is best within 3 m in trained sighted people and slightly further away in blind people.
Over about 20 ms, echolocation has largely ceased. Around 50 ms perceives the reflex of speech as a new separate sound - an echo. This means that in the interval between 20 to 50 ms, the reflex is experienced as a positive addition to the direct sound. Between 20 - 50 ms there is no exact spatial preception of the room as in echolocation.
The listening tests for Tools/Olive's spinorama in Harman's special listening room have a lateral delay of about 20 ms and an attenuation of about 8 dB.
This is not a coincidence as Toole/Olive published an article with a review of the publications in the area and that they carried out their own studies that confirmed the optimal time and attenuation of reflections in rooms.
Unfortunately, few meet these variables.